I thought this comic was not only amusing, but fit perfectly with
something that James Sosnoski mentioned in Hyper-readers
and Their Reading Engines: “Because readers characteristically navigate
textual landscapes by searching them for key words and thus often omitting
passages that do not “match,” hyper-reading will be labeled “subjective,” “superficial,”
and “de-contextualized’” (164). Of course, Sosnoski didn’t take this particular
stance in his paper; he was merely pointing out a common perception of
hypertext. In fact, Sosnoski asserted, “[I]n my account, hyper-reading is a
rewarding experience because it extends my ability to read” (165). I am
intrigued though, by this concept of “meaning-depraved information overload.”
On one hand, I am tempted, like the boy in the cartoon, to discount all
the “visual extras” available through technology, as if anything other than
text is somehow less authoritative or meaninful. On the other hand, I also remember that
Eva-Maria Jakobs (author of The Evolution
of Web-Site Genres) wrote, “What exactly does the term hypertext refer to?...The content of a mode can also be presented
in a variety of ways (as text, graphics, animation, video, audio). The choice
of content is dependent on the author’s intentions—hypertext nodes can, for
example, consist solely of technical drawings” (357). I think one of the big
issues many people have with hyper-text is the result of hyper-reading, which
Sosnoski said is characterized by filtering, skimming, pecking, imposing,
filming, traspassing, de-authorizing, and fragmenting (163). When I first read
that list, I immediately associated it with phrases like “diminished meaning”
and “compromised integrity.” Like Sosnoski admitted, I also feel guilty about
skimming text in order to get the general idea; I hold the idea that in order
to really know the material, one should read
the entire text. I try to tell myself that I’m maintaining authorial “integrity”
on research papers and critical essays if I am able to understand and properly
cite—in context—the work of another writer. However, I have since realized that
no matter how hard I try, my interpretation will, at some point or another,
deviate from author intent. Sosnoski pointed out, “[R]eaders can raid the texts
uncovered by their search results in order to assemble their details as ANOTHER
text which is, so to speak, re-authored by the reader” (166). The meaning will
inadvertedly change from writer to reader, and reader to writer, as both are
involved in the composition/invention process.
In my earlier
prejudice about the “meaninglessness” of hypertext, I forgot that it is indeed
similar to the reading conventions I take for granted. For example, according to
Jakobs:
This approach
views hypertext as a principle of presentation that over time has emerged from
the experimental stage and has led to a broad range of conventional pragmatic
patterns, most of which have been the focus of linguistic research. On the
other hand, these patterns represent a considerable proportion of everyday
communicative strategies. We use them in dealing with the Internet: in looking
for information, doing one’s banking, ordering books from Amazon, looking up
items in Wikipedia, reading online newspapers, booking vacations or putting
resources for students online. (356)
Sosnoski also
compared a search engine to reading the index or table of contents of an
encyclopedia to find a specific, desired topic. Despite my initial denial, I do
employ certain selective reading skills whether I’m searching for a library
book or browsing on the internet. My point is, I shouldn’t discount hypertext
as being “meaningless” just because its appearance is different—especially since
meaning in large part has to do with the reader, the one interpreting a work. “By
framing texts, readers assimilate them to their interests and hence render them
significant in the context of their concerns,” observed Sosnoski. His words are
something I should do well to remember as I navigate through hypertext as a new
outlet for writing.
I can understand why someone would dislike hyper-text because hyper-reading is the result. However I personally do not have a problem with hyper-reading, in fact I actually prefer it sometimes. I know you can miss points, incorrectly label, or de-contextualize a text as you brought up earlier, but if you are trained to read in this manner you rarely make these errors. Since high school I have been learning how to hyper-read. It started with a SAT tutor that taught me to read certain parts of a text, which indicate how the paragraphs are going to flow. Also in studying for the LSATs all the practice material tells students to read by looking for certain words that indicate changes in the article. And honestly I’m not sure how you would complete a passage without hyper-reading, you don’t have enough time: 35 minutes for 27 questions.
ReplyDelete